Comment: Is China's one-child policy to blame for personality changes?

A study shows people born in China under the one-child policy are less trusting, trustworthy, competitive, conscientious and optimistic than those born prior to its implementation.

china_baby_drinking_2509_B_getty_951963065
By

People born in China under the (OCP) – a policy applied since 1979, restricting urban couples to having only one child – are less trusting, trustworthy, competitive, conscientious, risk-seeking, and optimistic than those born prior to its implementation.

At least, that was the finding of a by Australian economists in Science Express.

With this in mind, imagine you are one of the many individuals born under the OCP. According to the research, you are one of the , a , privileged and pampered by indulgent parents. You have several undesirable psychological characteristics, and the OCP is to blame.

Now, imagine a different scenario. You are a Chinese policy-maker being lobbied to abolish the OCP. You've endured years of bleating about the appalling consequences of the policy, such as resulting from abortions of baby girls and the increasing proportion of elderly people with fewer offspring to care for them.

You're now told that those born just after the introduction of OCP – who are now in their thirties – perform more poorly on a range of psychological measures than those born pre-OCP, just a few years older than them.

Both individuals would be justified in asking for evidence: these are, after all, strong claims with significant implications.

But is the evidence compelling or merely suggestive? Suggestive data are fine if conclusions drawn from them are suitably restrained. In the Scottish philosopher 's words: “a wise man […] proportions his belief to the evidence.”





















hryrjrzn-1363308777.jpg
Saf'





















Using a range of economic games involving decisions about giving and investing money, and sophisticated , the authors of the Science Express paper – from Monash University, ANU and the University of Melbourne – drew the conclusions referred to above: those born prior to the OCP (1975 or 1978 for the purposes of the study) are more trusting, trustworthy, competitive, conscientious, risk-seeking, and optimistic than those born under the OCP (1980 or 1983).

The individual born under the OCP may want to ask: is there really a difference between those born under OCP versus pre-OCP? The policy-maker will want to know whether the OCP really caused this difference.

Taking the Science Express article at face value, the answers appear to be “yes” and “yes”. Group differences (for example, OCP participants invested 58% of a hypothetical sum versus 66% in the non-OCP group) are adorned with asterisks – the more, it seems, the better.

The term “causal” is used five times and “impact” ten – these are , describing a relation that is much more precise than mere association. The results, at first glance, could hardly be clearer.

Same but different

But are there really differences between the people studied? In simplified terms, a result in social science is typically called “” if the probability of the result occurring merely by chance, rather than reflecting a real difference, is low. The widely-accepted criterion is 5%.

In the Science Express paper, when controlling for relevant variables to rule out alternative explanations, four of the five differences in behavioural outcomes (trust, trustworthiness, risk-taking, competitiveness, but not altruism) are labelled “significant”, but using a criterion of 10%.

Had the widely-accepted 5% cut-off been used, two of the outcomes (trustworthiness and competitiveness) would not have been labelled significant.

Although the cut-off is largely arbitrary and somewhat flexible, the fact remains that for two of the outcomes, a conclusion of “no difference” is admissible by widely-accepted standards.

And invoking – an alternative way to compare groups – does little counter this: the effect sizes for trustworthiness and competitiveness are smaller than for trust and risk-taking. In short, some results are marginal at best.





















x5fpnjvv-1363308517.jpg
Jzimbabwe





















Alternative realities

So can the supposed differences in trust, trustworthiness, competitiveness, conscientiousness, risk-seeking, and optimism be attributed to the OCP?

The authors controlled for a range of variables in order to rule out alternatives. They also correctly describe the study as a , which is to say their groups are naturally occurring rather than randomly allocated.

The problem with natural experiments is that alternative explanations cannot be easily dismissed.

Tobacco companies understand this all too well: “the deceased did smoke our cigarettes and die from lung cancer, Your Honour, but was also exposed to asbestos …”

Perhaps the OCP is the most likely explanation for the group differences, but are there alternatives?

The bigger picture

China experienced dramatic societal change over the period in question. The ended around 1976 and triggered the opening of China's economy. China established diplomatic ties with the United States in the very year the OCP was introduced. In other words, a lot was going on.





















k464fm3s-1363310819.jpg
Saf'





















The extent to which the authors statistically control variables purported to assess these alternatives (e.g. attitudes to government intervention as an indicator of the effect of the opening of the economy) is admirable, but involves many assumptions.

The OCP may be the most likely explanation of the group differences, but alternative explanations are not completely precluded by the data.

So, are there really differences in these psychological variables between those born pre- and post-OCP? Yes, but some are marginal. Can these differences be attributed to OCP? Perhaps – although several plausible alternative explanations cannot be dismissed.

The push for the abolition of OCP can gain momentum if strong evidence for its negative implications is discovered. Strong claims based on merely suggestive evidence from a single study on a narrow sample will convince nobody with influence, and perhaps only offend a population of young Chinese-born individuals who must wear the “little emperor” label.

The conclusions, to reiterate Hume, are not yet proportional to the evidence.

Dan Costa does not work for, consult to, own shares in or receive funding from any company or organisation that would benefit from this article, and has no relevant affiliations.

count.gif

Share
Published 18 March 2013 2:10pm
Updated 3 September 2013 6:04pm
Source: The Conversation

Share this with family and friends


Get SBS News daily and direct to your Inbox

Sign up now for the latest news from Australia and around the world direct to your inbox.

By subscribing, you agree to SBS’s terms of service and privacy policy including receiving email updates from SBS.

Download our apps
SBS News
SBS Audio
SBS On Demand

Listen to our podcasts
An overview of the day's top stories from SBS News
Interviews and feature reports from SBS News
Your daily ten minute finance and business news wrap with SBS Finance Editor Ricardo Gonçalves.
A daily five minute news wrap for English learners and people with disability
Get the latest with our News podcasts on your favourite podcast apps.

Watch on SBS
SBS World News

SBS World News

Take a global view with Australia's most comprehensive world news service
Watch the latest news videos from Australia and across the world